Take a look at the Recent articles

Balancing a book on the head affects postural control

Eva-Maj Malmström

1Department of Neurology and Rehabilitation Medicine, Section for specialized pain Rehabilitation, Clinical Sciences Lund University, Skåne University Hospital, S-221 85 Lund, Sweden

Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Clinical Sciences Lund University, Skåne University Hospital, S-221 85 Lund, Sweden

Martin Kronkvist

Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Clinical Sciences Lund University, Skåne University Hospital, S-221 85 Lund, Sweden

Fredrik Tjernström

Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Clinical Sciences Lund University, Skåne University Hospital, S-221 85 Lund, Sweden

E-mail : Fredrik.Tjernstrom@med.lu.se

DOI: 10.15761/OHNS.1000133

Article
Article Info
Author Info
Figures & Data

Abstract

An emergent line in postural rehabilitation of dizzy patients is the use of various technical devices, based on creating external feedback postural cues. In this paper we evaluated the effects on postural control of balancing a book on the head, a method available for almost everyone. We tested 12 healthy volunteers (7 males, 5 females), aged 20-45 years, with and without balancing a book on the head, standing on a force platform while being posturally perturbed by vibratory simulation to the calf-muscles of the leg. The test conditions balancing a book on the head or not, with eyes open or closed were randomized in order to account for simple order effects.

Balancing a book stabilized postural sway when the eyes were open, predominantly by reducing the anteroposterior sway path and by decreasing the slow compensatory postural adjustments <0.1Hz (p=0.002).

The stabilizing effect of balancing a book could possibly be attributed to either external sensory information or a diversion of attention from maintaining postural control towards external focus (i.e. balancing a book constitutes a cognitive task). The study shows that a simple task as balancing a book affects postural control and could be used in rehabilitation of patients with postural imbalance.

Key words

postural contro, rehabilitation, external focus, feed-back mechanisms

Introduction

Upright bipedal stance involve both feed-back and feed-forward mechanisms [1]. Feed-back control depends on sensory input (vision, vestibular and proprioceptive) that are processed, integrated and weighted to their relative importance and context in the central nervous system (CNS) [2]. Feed-forward mechanisms depend upon previous postural and sensory experience as well as anticipatory expectance and involve the concept of “internal models”, whose output consists of preformed neuromuscular strategies activated in given situations automatically or voluntarily (anticipated movements) [3].

Poor postural control, sometimes expressed as dizziness/imbalance, can most often and to some extent be rehabilitated through exercises. This has been demonstrated both in isolated sensory loss (e.g. unilateral vestibular deafferentation) [4] and in central nervous dysfunction (e.g. multisensory deficits) [5,6]. The traditional rehabilitation procedures entail physiotherapeutic exercises best exemplified by individualized vestibular rehabilitation, designed to enhance both other sensory cues and central vestibular compensation mechanisms [7]. Training with technical devices such as Vertiguardã [8], and the Wii-Fitã balance board [9] has been shown to enhance postural control in a number of different clinical diagnoses. The devices act by creating external feed-back cues, thus intensifying the intrinsic feed-back mechanisms, and in the end enhance postural corrections. The devices are commercially available but cost money and thus not available for all. Historically, a common method to correct bad posture was to let subjects balance a book on their head, inducing a more erect and straight posture. In theory, balancing a book on the head could also affect postural control in 2 ways; by diverting attention from standing still or through providing gravitational cues that might affect postural alignment and also postural control.

Postural control can be studied either by examining upright stance with or without postural perturbations. It is generally held that in order to evaluate the dynamics of the system (adaptation, disclose pathology) some sort of perturbation is needed [10]. The ability to adapt to postural disturbances such as vibratory stimulation of the calf muscles can be demonstrated by posturography [11,12]. Vibration applied to a muscle or a muscle tendon increases the firing of the muscle spindles, thus signaling that the muscle is being stretched [13]. The stimulated muscle responds with a reflexive contraction (tonic vibratory reflex) [14]. In healthy subjects, the repeated high-intensity vibration of the calf muscles induces greater body sway that usually decreases over time by adaptation [11].

The aim for the study was to evaluate whether postural control can be affected through the act of balancing a book on the head in healthy controls during vibratory postural perturbations.

Material and methods

We studied 12 healthy paid volunteers (7 male, 5 female) aged 20-45 years (mean 27 ± 7 years), weight 60-110 kg (mean 77.1 ± 13.6 kg), and with body length of 167-190 cm (mean 1.80 ± 0.1 m). None of the subjects had any experience of similar tests and were naive concerning the study protocol and the methods employed. No one had experienced any otoneurological, neurological, psychiatric, orthopedic or hearing disorder or was using any medication except oral contraceptives. Alcoholic beverages and sedative drugs were proscribed for 24 hours preceding the testing. Informed written consent was obtained from all the subjects, the experiments were done in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 2004, and ethical approval from the local ethical board was obtained.

Test conditions were randomized to account for simple order effects; with and without balancing a book on the head, and with eyes open (EO) and eyes closed (EC). The book measured 215x150x15mm and weighed 340 grams. Test procedure was uniform and the same examiner performed all tests. The subjects were instructed to stand erect but not at attention, with arms crossed over the chest and feet at an angle of about 30 degrees open to the front and with the heals approximately 3 cm apart on a force platform. For the open eyes condition, the subjects were able to fixate on a mark on the wall 1.5 meters away and at the level of the subject’s eyes. In between the tests the subjects were allowed to rest if they so wished. In order to minimize the external disturbances for the test subjects, the recordings were performed while the test subjects listened to the same classical music sequence relayed through headphones.

Method

Postural control was evaluated by perturbing bipedal stance while standing on a force platform (400x400x75 mm) equipped with six strain‑gauge sensors (Figure 1). Forces and torques actuated by the feet were recorded with six degrees of freedom by a custom-built force platform, with accuracy better than 0.5 N. The vibrations were applied to the muscles by two cylindrical vibrators (0.06 m long and 0.01 m in diameters), held in place with an elastic strap around each leg. The vibration amplitude was 1.0 mm amplitude at a constant frequency of 85 Hz. The vibratory stimulation was executed according to a computer controlled pseudorandom binary sequence (PRBS) schedule [15] for 205 seconds by turning on/off the vibratory stimulation. The vibratory stimulation of 200s was preceded by a period of 30s of quiet stance. Each recorded test was divided into quiet stance and 4 perturbation periods (30–80, 80–130, 130–180, and 180–230 seconds, respectively), in order to be able to analyze differences in induced postural sway over time.

Figure 1. Posturography test. Drawn figure of the posturography procedure. The graph depicts raw data of generated torque during quiets stance and the 4 periods of vibratory stimulation. The generated forces were divided depending on the speed of correctional movements around the frequency of 0.1Hz, thus distinguishing fast reflexive movements from postural adjustments.

The PRBS schedule was composed of stimulation shift periods with random duration between 0.8 - 6.4 seconds (yielding an effective bandwidth of 0.1-2.5 Hz). Thus, the designated PRBS stimuli covered a broad power spectrum and the randomized stimulation reduced the opportunity to make anticipative and pre-emptive adjustments. Each of the perturbation periods contained the same amplitude and duration of perturbing stimulus.

Data analysis

Torque was measured and the variance of the torque values was analyzed. Postural stability during quiet stance is commonly analyzed using force platforms and the movements of the centre of pressure (CoP), i.e., the point of application of the ground reaction force. Torque correspond to CoP and torque τ can be calculated from the formula τ = CoP · Fz; where Fz ≈ m · g; where m = the assessed subjects mass (in kg) and g = gravitational constant 9.81 (meter/s2). Hence, changes in recorded torque are equivalent to changes in CoP [16]. We present torque in the form of energy used towards the support surface to maintain stability [17], which in turn corresponds to the efficiency of standing [18]. Changes in recorded torque from the force platform correspond well to the actual body movements and posture changes induced by vibratory stimulus [19]. The formula for variance is given by;

i=sample, n=number of samples recorded during an analyzed period.

The torque variance values were normalized to account for anthropometric differences between the subjects, using the subject’s squared height and squared mass, as height and mass are key factors influencing the body sway recorded by a force platform [17,20]. The squared nature of the variance algorithm made it necessary to use normalization with squared parameters to achieve unit agreement.

In the data analysis, the variance of torque was divided into three categories, total, low (<0.10 Hz), and high frequency (>0.10 Hz) (Figure 1). A fifth-order digital Finite duration Impulse Response (FIR) filter [21], with filter components selected to avoid aliasing was used for spectral separation. The frequency cut-off level of 0.1 Hz was based on Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT) analysis of the sway composition under EO and EC conditions [22]. The frequency limit at 0.1 Hz was also based upon empirical tests on recorded body sway, which have shown that this frequency limit is efficient when separating between fast corrective movements to maintain balance, and the smooth corrective changes in the overall stance [23]. In order to be able to discern the direction of postural sway the analyses were separated into anteroposterior and lateral sway, since vibratory perturbation mainly generate body sway in the anteroposterior plane and thus constitute the major postural response.

Statistics

The data was analyzed using a repeated measures GLM univariate ANOVA (General Linear Model univariate Analysis of Variance) test on log-transformed values [24]. In the GLM analysis, p-values <0.05 were considered statistically significant. With the GLM ANOVA variables, “book on the head”, “vision (EO,EC)”, and “adaptation (difference between the different periods)”, were analyzed with regard to their individual and combined effect on the posturography data. Since the results suggested significant effects from different visual conditions, the study includes a post-hoc analysis in the different visual conditions, again by the use of GLM univariate ANOVA.

Results

None of the subjects dropped their book during the measurements, nor did any patient fall or needed to take a step in order to maintain balance. Vision improved postural stability in the high frequency range >0.1 Hz (p=0.015) during quiet standing (Figure 2 and Table 1). Balancing a book on the head during quiet stance had no significant effect on postural stability in quiet standing (Table 1).

Figure 2. Quiet stance posturography with and without balancing a book on the head. Presented values are the mean and standard error of mean (SEM).
black bars = no book, grey bars = balancing a book on the head

Table 1. GLM-ANOVA statistics of quiet stance data

 

Quiet stance

 

BOOK

 

VISION

 

BOOK

VISION

 

 

Anteroposterior sway

 

Total

p-value

0.472

0.341

0.596

F

0.6

1.0

0.2

 

<0.1 Hz

p-value

0.360

0.457

0.908

F

0.9

0.6

0.0

 

>0.1 Hz

p-value

0.657

0.015

0.278

F

0.2

8.7

0.278

 

 

Lateral sway

 

Total

p-value

0.105

0.777

0.491

F

3.2

0.1

0.5

 

<0.1 Hz

p-value

0.058

0.805

0.188

F

4.6

0.1

2.0

 

>0.1 Hz

p-value

0.143

0.712

0.739

F

2.5

0.1

0.1

During vibratory perturbation (Table 2 and Figure 3) visual cues (EO) significantly improved postural anteroposterior stability in all frequencies of movements (total p<0.001, <0.1Hz p=0.008, >0.1Hz p<0.001). Vision also reduced lateral sway in total (p=0.001) and >0.1Hz (p<0.001). Over time or from period to period the induced body sway in anteroposterior direction decreased in all frequencies of movement (p<0.001), but in lateral sway only in low frequency movements <0.1Hz (p=0.023). Balancing a book on the head combined with visual cues (EO) significantly improved postural stability in anteroposterior sway <0.1 Hz (p=0.002).

Table 2. GLM ANOVA statistics during the 4 periods of vibratory perturbation.

VIBRATORY

PERTURBATION

BOOK

VISION

PERIOD

BOOK

VISION

BOOK
PERIOD

VISION

PERIOD

BOOK

VISION

PERIOD

Antero

Posterior

Sway

Total

p-value

0.179

<0.001

<0.001

0.114

0.363

0.309

0.180

F

2.1

66.7

53.2

2.9

0.9

1.1

2.1

<0.1 Hz

p-value

0.263

0.008

<0.001

0.002

0.371

0.165

0.549

F

1.4

10.3

66.0

16.0

0.9

2.2

0.4

>0.1 Hz

p-value

0.287

<0.001

<0.001

0.606

0.465

0.053

0.342

F

1.3

90.7

29.2

0.3

0.6

4.7

1.0

Lateral

Sway

 

Total

p-value

0.082

0.001

0.121

0.969

0.101

0.197

0.622

F

3.7

20.3

2.8

0.0

3.2

1.9

0.3

<0.1 Hz

p-value

0.108

0.178

0.023

0.430

0.434

0.244

0.117

F

3.1

2.1

6.9

0.7

0.7

1.5

2.9

>0.1 Hz

p-value

0.225

<0.001

0.371

0.434

0.154

0.064

0.239

F

1,7

40.5

0.9

0.7

2.3

4.2

1.6

Figure 3. Posturography during the 4 periods of vibratory perturbation The post-hoc GLM ANOVA statistics are given in the figure, with the F-value within brackets. Presented values are the mean and standard error of mean (SEM). black bars = no book, grey bars = balancing a book on the head

The post-hoc analysis of the different visual conditions (Figure 2) revealed that balancing a book on the head improved postural stability during EO in total anteroposterior sway (p=0.032) and total lateral sway (p=0.020), with the majority of improvement in sway <0.1Hz in both anteroposterior (p=0.009) and lateral plane p=0.045).

Figure 4A-B depicts the area on the platform, upon which the balancing forces, from one subject, were actuated during eyes open condition and balancing a book (B) or not (A). The area over which the subject exerted correctional forces decreased considerably in the anteroposterior plane when standing with a book on the head.

Figure 4. Platform recording The area on the platform over which correctional forces were actuated during the vibratory perturbation in one subject during eyes open test. A= no book, B= balancing a book on the head

Discussion

Postural parameters were affected by balancing a book on the head. During postural perturbations (vibratory calf stimulation) the stability improved significantly when the subjects had a book on their heads, with less forces actuated on the platform i.e., less energy was spent while standing. The graph in figure 2 also suggest that balancing a book on the head also affect postural control in quiet stance (without perturbation) with and without visual cues, though the difference was not significant. The improvement might have been due to that the book’s weight provided an external gravity reference, or to that the balancing act diverted attention from maintaining postural control towards a focus on keeping the book on the head (i.e., balancing a book constituted a minor cognitive task), or both.  

As an external reference, it is possible that the book conveyed information about head movements since changes of the books pressure on the head would signal changes of the gravitation vector of the head and thus help the postural control system to attenuate body sway during a perturbation, as a kind of haptic control. Very much in the same way as the somatosensory system can provide a spatial reference frame for the postural control system by simply touching an object during postural perturbations, and thus relating to allocentric reference frames [25]. Both the task of maintaining postural control and balancing a book would have the aim to stabilize the head in space through a top-down representation [26], and possibly also explain why postural sway was predominantly affected by the book-balancing act during test conditions with eyes open.

Cognitive influence on postural control has been investigated during different conditions (dual-task, backwards counting, digital tasks etc.) with various results.  The theory is that a postural and a cognitive task effectuated at the same time compete for a finite cortical capacity, and that the task perceived as most important is given priority [27]. We did not measure any performance of the book-balancing act other than recording that none dropped their book, so in this sense the present study does not evaluate dual tasks. Generally it appears that cognitive tasks affect postural control negatively if it already is impaired [28,29], or if the cognitive capacity itself is reduced [22]. The impact of a cognitive task seems also to be relative to the difficulty of the task, where postural sway decreases with increasing cognitive demand [30,31]. Taken together if balancing of a book constitutes a cognitive task or a task given priority, the results would make sense, i.e. that postural sway decreased during an easy task (EO) but was unaffected during a more difficult postural task (EC). The consequences from failure of the book task, i.e. dropping the book, were not severe and as such the task would not be given priority during EC and thus may not have constituted an external focus (compared to the vibratory perturbation) during EC condition. The lack of “book-effect” during EC could also suggest more dependence on vestibular and somatosensory feedback cues than on book-gravity cues. For rehabilitation purposes the EO condition is similar to most conditions involved in daily activities.

2021 Copyright OAT. All rights reserv

The balancing of a book on the head primarily improved postural control when visual cues were available. The improvement consisted of a change into a more stable position and through a reduction of the smooth corrective body movements in overall stance. The area over which the CoP was exerted during perturbations changed when balancing a book. The anteroposterior swaypath became reduced and since there thus is greater distance to postural limits, where the CoP exceeds the base of support, the reduction of swaypath could entail a greater capacity for postural adjustments. There was no change of the reflexive (>0.1Hz) corrective body movements, suggesting that the book-balancing act did not affect fast corrective adjustments involved in feed-back control.

Postural control improved during the perturbations over time, which supports earlier results [32], and the improvement was most pronounced during eyes closed and in anteroposterior direction. Adaptation to postural perturbations is mostly seen in the reflexive, feed-back dependent quick movements >0.1Hz [32], and this capacity for adaptation was not augmented by balancing a book in any condition, nor frequency. It could be that any effects from the book were attenuated, since the adaptation to postural perturbations without a book already was great [32].

Conclusion

The study demonstrates that balancing a book on the head affects postural sway and reduces the energy spent while standing. The concept constitutes an easy and accessible method, which could be attractive from a postural rehabilitation perspective, though the effect on patients with poor postural control with imbalance and/or dizziness remains to be studied.

References

  1. Harrison LB, Sessions RB, Kies MS (2014) Site-specific principles of management of head and neck cancer. 4th edition. Section2.Wolters Kluwer Health. 544-545.
  2. Stefanyszyn MA, Hidayat AA, Pe'er JJ, Flanagan JC (1994) Lacrimal sac tumors. Ophthal Plast Reconstr Surg 10: 169-184. [Crossref]
  3. El-Sawy T, Frank SJ, Hanna E, Sniegowski M, Lai SY, et al. (2013) Multidisciplinary management of lacrimal sac/nasolacrimal duct carcinomas. Ophthal Plast Reconstr Surg 29: 454-457. [Crossref]
  4. Katircioglu YA, Altiparmak UE, Akmansu H, Ustün H, Zorlu F, et al. (2003) Squamous cell carcinoma of the lacrimal sac. Orbit 22: 151-153. [Crossref]
  5. Montalban A, Liétin B, Louvrier C, Russier M, Kemeny JL, et al. (2010) Malignant lacrimal sac tumors. Eur Ann Otorhinolaryngol Head Neck Dis 127: 165-172. [Crossref]
  6. Munro AJ (1995) An overview of randomised controlled trials of adjuvant chemotherapy in head and neck cancer. Br J Cancer 71: 83-91. [Crossref]
  7. El-Sayed S, Nelson N (1996) Adjuvant and adjunctive chemotherapy in the management of squamous cell carcinoma of the head and neck region. A meta-analysis of prospective and randomized trials. J Clin Oncol 14: 838-847. [Crossref]
  8. Tomura N, Hashimoto M, Sashi R, Hirano H, Kobayashi M, et al. (1996) Superselective angio-CT of brain tumors. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 17: 1073-1080. [Crossref]
  9. Hirai T, Korogi Y, Ono K, Maruoka K, Harada K, et al. (2001) Intraarterial chemotherapy or chemoembolization for locally advanced and/or recurrent hepatic tumors: evaluation of the feeding artery with an interventional CT system. Cardiovasc Intervent Radiol 24: 176-179. [Crossref]
  10. Miyayama S, Yamashiro M, Hattori Y, Orito N, Matsui K, et al. (2011) Usefulness of C-arm CT during superselective infusion chemotherapy for advanced head and neck carcinoma. J Med Imaging Radiat Oncol 55: 368-372. [Crossref]
  11. Yokoyama J, Ohba S, Fujimaki M, Anzai T, Kojima M, et al. (2014) Impact of intra-arterial chemotherapy including internal carotid artery for advanced paranasal sinus cancers involving the skull base. Br J Cancer. 111: 2229-2234. [Crossref]
  12. Ni C, D'Amico DJ, Fan CQ, Kuo PK (1982) Tumors of the lacrimal sac: a clinicopathological analysis of 82 cases. Int Ophthalmol Clin 22: 121-140. [Crossref]
  13. Suarez C, Llorente JL, Fernandez De Leon R, Maseda E, Lopez A (2004) Prognostic factors in sinonasal tumors involving the anterior skull base. Head Neck 26: 136-144. [Crossref]

Editorial Information

Editor-in-Chief

Chin-Lung Kuo
Director of the Department of Otolaryngology
Hsinchu Armed Forces Hospital, Hsinchu
Taiwan

Article Type

Research Article

Publication history

Received date: October 06, 2016
Accepted date: November 02, 2016
Published date: November 07, 2016

Copyright

© 2016 Malmström EM. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Citation

Malmström EM (2016) Balancing a book on the head affects postural control. Otorhinolaryngol Head Neck Sur,2: DOI: 10.15761/OHNS.1000133.

Corresponding author

Fredrik Tjernström

Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Clinical Sciences Lund University, Skåne University Hospital, S-221 85 Lund, Sweden, Tel: +46 46175849, Fax: +46 462110968.

E-mail : Fredrik.Tjernstrom@med.lu.se

Figure 1. Posturography test. Drawn figure of the posturography procedure. The graph depicts raw data of generated torque during quiets stance and the 4 periods of vibratory stimulation. The generated forces were divided depending on the speed of correctional movements around the frequency of 0.1Hz, thus distinguishing fast reflexive movements from postural adjustments.

Figure 2. Quiet stance posturography with and without balancing a book on the head. Presented values are the mean and standard error of mean (SEM).
black bars = no book, grey bars = balancing a book on the head

Figure 3. Posturography during the 4 periods of vibratory perturbation The post-hoc GLM ANOVA statistics are given in the figure, with the F-value within brackets. Presented values are the mean and standard error of mean (SEM). black bars = no book, grey bars = balancing a book on the head

Figure 4. Platform recording The area on the platform over which correctional forces were actuated during the vibratory perturbation in one subject during eyes open test. A= no book, B= balancing a book on the head

Table 1. GLM-ANOVA statistics of quiet stance data

 

Quiet stance

 

BOOK

 

VISION

 

BOOK

VISION

 

 

Anteroposterior sway

 

Total

p-value

0.472

0.341

0.596

F

0.6

1.0

0.2

 

<0.1 Hz

p-value

0.360

0.457

0.908

F

0.9

0.6

0.0

 

>0.1 Hz

p-value

0.657

0.015

0.278

F

0.2

8.7

0.278

 

 

Lateral sway

 

Total

p-value

0.105

0.777

0.491

F

3.2

0.1

0.5

 

<0.1 Hz

p-value

0.058

0.805

0.188

F

4.6

0.1

2.0

 

>0.1 Hz

p-value

0.143

0.712

0.739

F

2.5

0.1

0.1

Table 2. GLM ANOVA statistics during the 4 periods of vibratory perturbation.

VIBRATORY

PERTURBATION

BOOK

VISION

PERIOD

BOOK

VISION

BOOK
PERIOD

VISION

PERIOD

BOOK

VISION

PERIOD

Antero

Posterior

Sway

Total

p-value

0.179

<0.001

<0.001

0.114

0.363

0.309

0.180

F

2.1

66.7

53.2

2.9

0.9

1.1

2.1

<0.1 Hz

p-value

0.263

0.008

<0.001

0.002

0.371

0.165

0.549

F

1.4

10.3

66.0

16.0

0.9

2.2

0.4

>0.1 Hz

p-value

0.287

<0.001

<0.001

0.606

0.465

0.053

0.342

F

1.3

90.7

29.2

0.3

0.6

4.7

1.0

Lateral

Sway

 

Total

p-value

0.082

0.001

0.121

0.969

0.101

0.197

0.622

F

3.7

20.3

2.8

0.0

3.2

1.9

0.3

<0.1 Hz

p-value

0.108

0.178

0.023

0.430

0.434

0.244

0.117

F

3.1

2.1

6.9

0.7

0.7

1.5

2.9

>0.1 Hz

p-value

0.225

<0.001

0.371

0.434

0.154

0.064

0.239

F

1,7

40.5

0.9

0.7

2.3

4.2

1.6